Events

Mo Tu We Th Fr Sa Su
49 01 02 03 04 05 06 07
50 08 09 10 11 12 13 14
51 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
52 22 23 24 25 26 27 28
1 29 30 31 01 02 03 04

» All Events

Print page

Perceptual changes – a key to our consciousness

Thursday 18.11.2010 18:00

By: Stephanie Bertenbreiter

Tübingen scientists use image processing effects to decipher brain functions


Dichoptic stimulus. Different pictures are presented to the eyes to provoke percept changes (binocular rivalry). Picture: Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen. Tübingen, November18th, 2010.
With his coat billowing behind him and his right eye tightly closed, Captain Blackbeard watches the endless sea with his telescope. Suddenly the sea disappears as the pirate opens his right eye. The only thing he sees is his hand holding the telescope. And then, a moment later, the sea is back again. What happened was a change in perception. Our brain usually combines the two slightly divergent images of our eyes into a single consistent perception. However, if the visual information does not match, only one image is seen at a time. This phenomenon is called "binocular rivalry". Researchers around Andreas Bartels at the Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neurosciences (CIN) and the Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics in Tübingen, Germany used this phenomenon to decipher a key mechanism of the brain functions that contributes to conscious visual perception. (Current Biology, November 18th, 2010)  

We do not consciously perceive everything around us, even if it falls into our field of vision. The overwhelming abundance of information forces our brain to focus on a few important things; our perception is an ongoing process of selecting, grouping and interpreting visual information. Even though we have two eyes, our brain combines the two impressions. Experts call this binocular vision. Yet, if conflicting information is presented to the eyes, only the input to one eye is perceived at a time, while the other is suppressed. Our perception changes at specific intervals between the two images - a phenomenon called “binocular rivalry”. This process occurs automatically without voluntary control.  

The scientists, Natalia Zaretskaya, Axel Thielscher, Nikos Logothetis and Andreas Bartels demonstrated that the frequency at which alternations between the visual information occurred could be experimentally reduced: Two different stimuli, a house and a face, were projected into the right and left eyes, respectively, of 15 experimental subjects. Since the brain could not match the pictures, alternations in perception occurred. When the scientists temporarily applied an alternating magnetic field to the subjects’ posterior parietal cortex, a higher-order area of the brain, the perception of each individual image was prolonged.  

“Our findings suggest that the parietal cortex is causally involved in selecting the information that is consciously perceived,” explains Natalia Zaretskaya, a Ph.D. student involved in the project. „It also demonstrates the important role of this area in visual awareness.”  

“Understanding the neural circuits underlying the percepts and their switches might give us some insight into how consciousness is implemented in the brain, or at least into the dynamic processes underlying it“, explains Andreas Bartels, scientist at the CIN.  


Original publication:
Natalia Zaretskaya, Axel Thielscher, Nikos K. Logothetis, Andreas Bartels: Disrupting parietal function prolongs dominance durations in binocular rivalry, Current Biology (2010); doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.046  

Contact:
Dr. Andreas Bartels
Phone: +49 7071 601-656
E-mail: andreas.bartels[at]cin-uni-tuebingen.de  

Stephanie Bertenbreiter (Public Relations Office)
Phone: +49 7071 601-472
E-mail: presse[at]tuebingen.mpg.de      

The Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics works in the elucidation of cognitive processes. It employs 325 people from more than 40 countries and is located at the Max Planck Campus in Tübingen, Germany. The Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics is one of 80 research institutes that the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science maintains in Germany and abroad.  

The Werner Reichardt Centre for Integrative Neuroscience (CIN) is an interdisciplinary institution at the Eberhard Karls University Tübingen. Several faculties, the Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, the Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, and the Fraunhofer Institute for Manufacturing, Engineering and Automation are part of the CIN, whose interdisciplinary concept is supported by numerous other internal and external partners. The CIN scientists strive to further our understanding of the brain’s capabilities such as perception, memory, communication and action and how brain diseases impair functions.
Dichoptic stimulus. Different pictures are presented to the eyes to provoke percept changes (binocular rivalry). Picture: Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen.
Dichoptic stimulus. Different pictures are presented to the eyes to provoke percept changes (binocular rivalry). Picture: Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen.
Proband seated in a confortable chair, with her chin on a chin rest conducting an experiment. A CRT monitor with shutter glasses was used for the dichoptic stimulus presentation. Picture: Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen.
Proband seated in a confortable chair, with her chin on a chin rest conducting an experiment. A CRT monitor with shutter glasses was used for the dichoptic stimulus presentation. Picture: Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics Tübingen.